Encyc

Encyc houses over 100 concepts relevant to the history of eugenics and its continued implications in contemporary life. These entries represent in-depth explorations of key concepts for understanding eugenics.

Aboriginal and Indigenous Peoples
Michael Billinger
Alcoholism and drug use
Paula Larsson
Archives and institutions
Mary Horodyski
Assimilation
Karen Stote
Bioethical appeals to eugenics
Tiffany Campbell
Bioethics
Gregor Wolbring
Birth control
Molly Ladd-Taylor
Childhood innocence
Joanne Faulkner
Colonialism
Karen Stote
Conservationism
Michael Kohlman
Criminality
Amy Samson
Degeneracy
Michael Billinger
Dehumanization: psychological aspects
David Livingstone Smith
Deinstitutionalization
Erika Dyck
Developmental disability
Dick Sobsey
Disability rights
Joshua St. Pierre
Disability, models of
Gregor Wolbring
Down Syndrome
Michael Berube
Education
Erna Kurbegovic
Education as redress
Jonathan Chernoguz
Educational testing
Michelle Hawks
Environmentalism
Douglas Wahlsten
Epilepsy
Frank W. Stahnisch
Ethnicity and race
Michael Billinger
Eugenic family studies
Robert A. Wilson
Eugenic traits
Robert A. Wilson
Eugenics
Robert A. Wilson
Eugenics as wrongful
Robert A. Wilson
Eugenics: positive vs negative
Robert A. Wilson
Family planning
Caroline Lyster
Farming and animal breeding
Sheila Rae Gibbons
Feeble-mindedness
Wendy Kline
Feminism
Esther Rosario
Fitter family contests
Molly Ladd-Taylor
Gender
Caroline Lyster
Genealogy
Leslie Baker
Genetic counseling
Gregor Wolbring
Genetics
James Tabery
Genocide
Karen Stote
Guidance clinics
Amy Samson
Hereditary disease
Sarah Malanowski
Heredity
Michael Billinger
Human enhancement
Gregor Wolbring
Human experimentation
Frank W. Stahnisch
Human nature
Chris Haufe
Huntington's disease
Alice Wexler
Immigration
Jacalyn Ambler
Indian--race-based definition
Karen Stote
Informed consent
Erika Dyck
Institutionalization
Erika Dyck
Intellectual disability
Licia Carlson
Intelligence and IQ testing
Aida Roige
KEY CONCEPTS
Robert A. Wilson
Kant on eugenics and human nature
Alan McLuckie
Marriage
Alexandra Minna Stern
Masturbation
Paula Larsson
Medicalization
Gregor Wolbring
Mental deficiency: idiot, imbecile, and moron
Wendy Kline
Miscegenation
Michael Billinger
Motherhood
Molly Ladd-Taylor
Natural and artificial selection
Douglas Wahlsten
Natural kinds
Matthew H. Slater
Nature vs nurture
James Tabery
Nazi euthanasia
Paul Weindling
Nazi sterilization
Paul Weindling
Newgenics
Caroline Lyster
Nordicism
Michael Kohlman
Normalcy and subnormalcy
Gregor Wolbring
Parenting and newgenics
Caroline Lyster
Parenting of children with disabilities
Dick Sobsey
Parenting with intellectual disabilities
David McConnell
Pauperism
Caroline Lyster
Person
Gregor Wolbring
Physician assisted suicide
Caroline Lyster
Political science and race
Dexter Fergie
Popular culture
Colette Leung
Population control
Alexandra Stern
Prenatal testing
Douglas Wahlsten
Project Prevention
Samantha Balzer
Propaganda
Colette Leung
Psychiatric classification
Steeves Demazeux
Psychiatry and mental health
Frank W. Stahnisch
Psychology
Robert A. Wilson
Public health
Lindsey Grubbs
Race and racialism
Michael Billinger
Race betterment
Erna Kurbegovic
Race suicide
Adam Hochman
Racial hygiene
Frank W. Stahnisch
Racial hygiene and Nazism
Frank Stahnisch
Racial segregation
Paula Larsson
Racism
Michael Billinger
Reproductive rights
Erika Dyck
Reproductive technologies
Caroline Lyster
Residential schools
Faun Rice
Roles of science in eugenics
Robert A. Wilson
Schools for the Deaf and Deaf Identity
Bartlomiej Lenart
Science and values
Matthew J. Barker
Selecting for disability
Clarissa Becerra
Sexual segregation
Leslie Baker
Sexuality
Alexandra Minna Stern
Social Darwinism
Erna Kurbegovic
Sociobiology
Robert A. Wilson
Sorts of people
Robert A. Wilson
Special education
Jason Ellis
Speech-language pathology
Joshua St. Pierre
Standpoint theory
Joshua St. Pierre
Sterilization
Wendy Kline
Sterilization compensation
Paul Weindling
Stolen generations
Joanne Faulkner
Subhumanization
Licia Carlson
Today and Tomorrow: To-day and To-morrow book series
Michael Kohlman
Training schools for the feeble-minded
Katrina Jirik
Trans
Aleta Gruenewald
Transhumanism and radical enhancement
Mark Walker
Tuberculosis
Maureen Lux
Twin Studies
Douglas Wahlsten & Frank W. Stahnisch
Ugly Laws
Susan M. Schweik and Robert A. Wilson
Unfit, the
Cameron A.J. Ellis
Violence and disability
Dick Sobsey
War
Frank W. Stahnisch
Women's suffrage
Sheila Rae Gibbons

Roles of science in eugenics

The relationship of eugenics to science is intricate and many-layered, starting with Sir Francis Galton’s original definition of eugenics as “the science of improving stock”. Eugenics was originally conceived of not only as a science by many of its proponents, but as a new, meliorative science emerging from findings of a range of nascent sciences, including anthropology and criminology in the late 19th-century, and genetics and psychiatry in the early 20th-century. Although during the years between the two World Wars many central claims made by eugenicists were critiqued by scientists in these disciplines, in more recent years forms of eugenics (e.g., liberal eugenics”) have been defended as an inevitable outcome of biotechnologies and respect for autonomous choice. Understanding the shifting and varied roles that science has played in eugenics requires an appreciation of the ways in which science and values are intertwined.

Science and Eugenics: The Late Nineteenth-Century
When Galton coined the term “eugenics” in his Inquiries into the Human Faculty, he characterized it as “the science of improving stock … to give the more suitable races or strains of blood a better chance of prevailing over the less suitable” (1883: 24-25). Galton identified eugenics as an explicatively meliorative enterprise, one concerned with improvement of some kind within a population that contains the “more suitable” and the “less suitable”. As such, the science of eugenics presupposes questions about value: which races or strains of blood are more suitable, and which less suitable, and why?

Galton’s earlier studies Hereditary Genius (1869) and English Men of Science (1874) made it clear that the more suitable “strains of blood” were those found in family lineages with high levels of social and professional accomplishment. Part of Galton’s own aim was to establish grounds for the view that “talents” and “character”, both thought of as mental traits of people, were subject as much to hereditary transmission as were physical traits. In pursuit of that goal, Galton both drew on, and made significant contributions to, statistical techniques for studying populations, such as regression and the analysis of covariance (Mackenzie 1981). The reliance of early work in eugenics on social statistics, measurement, and analysis, contributed to a view of eugenics as scientific.

Eugenic views of some of these “less suitable” races and “strains of blood” were incorporated directly from common perceptions of certain groups of people who represented social problems for late 19th-century Western societies: the poor, the alcoholic, the feeble-minded. These sorts of people were viewed as themselves inferior in some intrinsic way, and so responsible for the corresponding problems of pauperism, alcoholism, and feeble-mindedness. Eugenic interventions directed at them were thus seen as solutions to those problems. Eugenic family studies, beginning with Richard Dugdale’s “The Jukes” in 1877, as well as hereditary views of criminality, were taken to provide a scientific basis for sexual segregation and sterilization policies, as well as for broader eugenic thinking about future generations (Rafter 1988, 2008).

The “less suitable” also included, as Galton made clear, certain “races”. Here folk views that reflected nineteenth-century racist biases were incorporated into and reinforced by science, in this case the science of anthropology. Eugenics took racial categories, such as “Black” and “Indian”, as biologically distinct kinds of people, each associated with a suit of different characteristics that made them more or less suitable for civilization and its requirements. The German term “Rassenhygiene”, introduced in 1895 by Alfred Ploetz in Germany and sometimes translated as “racial hygiene” was, in essence the term for eugenics, and came to be associated through Ploetz with the superiority of the Aryan race in twentieth-century Nazism.

Eugenics and Science Come of Age: The Early Twentieth-Century
If anthropology, criminology, and the development of social statistics provided the scientific grounding for the origins of eugenics, genetics, psychology, and psychiatry can be taken as the sciences that facilitated the transition of eugenics from the realm of ideas to social policy. Although “genetics” was coined by William Bateson only in 1906, the idea that eugenic traits ran in families was typically underwritten by some kind of appeal to hereditary material that was passed down a family lineage. Likewise, the idea that mental defectiveness or feeble-mindedness was at the root of what made people “less suitable” was given its scientific grounding in psychology and psychiatry through the rise of psychological testing, the regimentation of categories such as “imbecile” and “moron”, and the development of formal manuals of psychiatric classification.

Much of the scientific work here was undertaken by institutions established as part of the eugenics movement. For example, the Eugenics Records Office at Cold Spring Harbor in the United States, and the Kaiser Wilhelm Institute for Anthropology, Human Heredity, and Eugenics in Berlin, Germany, both supported scientific research that collected statistics on traits putatively running in families. Scientists at the ERO significantly expanded both the list of traits and the kind of people that fell within the purview of eugenics, including Hebrews, Greeks, Slovenians and other ethnic and national groups as “races”, and subjecting traits such as thalassophilia (love of the sea) and rebelliousness, to eugenic analysis (Allen 1986).

The scientific credibility of both the research and research personnel were influential in policies governing immigration, education, and the lives of Indigenous people. For example, Harry Laughlin from the Eugenics Record Office was an expert witness to the committee that shepherded the restrictive Immigration Restriction Act in the US in 1924. In Canada, eugenicists with influential scientific authority included the psychiatrist Charles Kirk Clark, who linked feeble-mindedness and mental deficiency in Canada to immigration from southern and eastern Europe, and Helen MacMurchy, the first woman to graduate from medicine at the University of Toronto, who became Ontario’s “Inspector of the Feeble-Minded” in 1915.

Scientific Critiques of Eugenics
The best-known early scientific critiques of eugenics focused on the genetic assumptions made by the research undertaken by biometricians in the United Kingdom and by researchers at the Eugenics Record Office in the United States. These were challenges from within the developing science of genetics, and argued that (a) sterilization was unlikely to be an effective form of negative eugenics (Jennings 1931), (b) many eugenic traits, including most notably “feeble-mindedness” had a dubious genetic basis, with eugenic directives based on a simplistic and mistaken form of genetic determinism (Penrose 1949), and (c) racialized traits and race itself did not have the populational genetic bases that would justify the role given to them within the eugenics movement (Dunn and Dobzhansky 1946). These critiques of eugenics chiefly during the interwar years have been associated with a shift from “mainline” to “reform” eugenics by the historian Daniel Kevles (1985). On Kevles’ view, science offered a self-correction to an enthusiastic and uncritical application of biological knowledge to questions of human enhancement, leading to a form of postwar eugenics divorced from racism and genetic determinism.

Newgenics
Although emerging political critiques of eugenic practices, such as sterilization and segregation, certainly played a role in the virtual disappearance of explicit support for a science called “eugenics” following the end of the Second World War, Kevles’ view provides one way to reconcile that disappearance with continuing interest in using scientific knowledge and technology for intergenerational human improvement. That interest is manifest in a variety of contemporary contexts that are sometimes characterized as new forms of eugenics, or “newgenics”. These include policies aimed at containing the reproductive rates of populous countries such as India and China stemming from fears of sustainability (Connolly 2008), and advances in prenatal testing that are coupled with the practice of selective abortion, particularly of fetuses deemed likely to develop a disability (Asch 2003).

-Robert A. Wilson

  • Allen, Garland, 1986, “The Eugenics Record Office at Cold Spring Harbor, 1910-1940: An Essay in Institutional History”, Osiris 2, pp.225-264.

  • Asch, Adrienne, 2003, “Disability Equality and Prenatal Testing: Contradictory or Compatible?”, Florida State University Law Review 30, pp. 315-342.

  • Connolly, Matthew, 2008, Fatal Misconception: The Struggle to Control World Population. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.

  • Dunn, L.C., and T. Dobzhansky, 1946, Heredity, Race, and Society. New York: New American Library.

  • Galton, Francis, 1869, Hereditary Genius: An Inquiry into its Laws and Consequences. London: Macmillan.

  • Galton, Francis, 1874, English Men of Science: Their Nature and Nurture. London: Macmillan.

  • Galton, Francis, 1883, Inquiries into Human Faculty and its Development. London: Macmillan.

  • Jennings, Herbert Spencer, 1931, “Eugenics” in Edwin Seligman and Alvin Johnson (eds.), Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences, volume 5. New York: Macmillan.

  • Kevles, Daniel, 1985, In the Name of Eugenics. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  • Penrose, Lionel, 1949, The Biology of Mental Defect. London: Sidgwick and Jackson.

  • Rafter, Nicole Hahn, 1988, White Trash: The Eugenic Family Studies, 1977-1919”. Boston: Northeastern University Press.

  • Rafter, Nicole, 2008, The Criminal Brain: Understanding Biological Theories of Crime. New York: New York University Press.

  • Roll-Hansen, Nils, 2010, “Eugenics and the Science of Genetics”, in Alison Bashford and Philippa Levine (eds), The Oxford Handbook of the History of Eugenics. New York: Oxford University Press, pp.80-97.