Encyc

Encyc houses over 100 concepts relevant to the history of eugenics and its continued implications in contemporary life. These entries represent in-depth explorations of key concepts for understanding eugenics.

Aboriginal and Indigenous Peoples
Michael Billinger
Alcoholism and drug use
Paula Larsson
Archives and institutions
Mary Horodyski
Assimilation
Karen Stote
Bioethical appeals to eugenics
Tiffany Campbell
Bioethics
Gregor Wolbring
Birth control
Molly Ladd-Taylor
Childhood innocence
Joanne Faulkner
Colonialism
Karen Stote
Conservationism
Michael Kohlman
Criminality
Amy Samson
Degeneracy
Michael Billinger
Dehumanization: psychological aspects
David Livingstone Smith
Deinstitutionalization
Erika Dyck
Developmental disability
Dick Sobsey
Disability rights
Joshua St. Pierre
Disability, models of
Gregor Wolbring
Down Syndrome
Michael Berube
Education
Erna Kurbegovic
Education as redress
Jonathan Chernoguz
Educational testing
Michelle Hawks
Environmentalism
Douglas Wahlsten
Epilepsy
Frank W. Stahnisch
Ethnicity and race
Michael Billinger
Eugenic family studies
Robert A. Wilson
Eugenic traits
Robert A. Wilson
Eugenics
Robert A. Wilson
Eugenics as wrongful
Robert A. Wilson
Eugenics: positive vs negative
Robert A. Wilson
Family planning
Caroline Lyster
Farming and animal breeding
Sheila Rae Gibbons
Feeble-mindedness
Wendy Kline
Feminism
Esther Rosario
Fitter family contests
Molly Ladd-Taylor
Gender
Caroline Lyster
Genealogy
Leslie Baker
Genetic counseling
Gregor Wolbring
Genetics
James Tabery
Genocide
Karen Stote
Guidance clinics
Amy Samson
Hereditary disease
Sarah Malanowski
Heredity
Michael Billinger
Human enhancement
Gregor Wolbring
Human experimentation
Frank W. Stahnisch
Human nature
Chris Haufe
Huntington's disease
Alice Wexler
Immigration
Jacalyn Ambler
Indian--race-based definition
Karen Stote
Informed consent
Erika Dyck
Institutionalization
Erika Dyck
Intellectual disability
Licia Carlson
Intelligence and IQ testing
Aida Roige
KEY CONCEPTS
Robert A. Wilson
Kant on eugenics and human nature
Alan McLuckie
Marriage
Alexandra Minna Stern
Masturbation
Paula Larsson
Medicalization
Gregor Wolbring
Mental deficiency: idiot, imbecile, and moron
Wendy Kline
Miscegenation
Michael Billinger
Motherhood
Molly Ladd-Taylor
Natural and artificial selection
Douglas Wahlsten
Natural kinds
Matthew H. Slater
Nature vs nurture
James Tabery
Nazi euthanasia
Paul Weindling
Nazi sterilization
Paul Weindling
Newgenics
Caroline Lyster
Nordicism
Michael Kohlman
Normalcy and subnormalcy
Gregor Wolbring
Parenting and newgenics
Caroline Lyster
Parenting of children with disabilities
Dick Sobsey
Parenting with intellectual disabilities
David McConnell
Pauperism
Caroline Lyster
Person
Gregor Wolbring
Physician assisted suicide
Caroline Lyster
Political science and race
Dexter Fergie
Popular culture
Colette Leung
Population control
Alexandra Stern
Prenatal testing
Douglas Wahlsten
Project Prevention
Samantha Balzer
Propaganda
Colette Leung
Psychiatric classification
Steeves Demazeux
Psychiatry and mental health
Frank W. Stahnisch
Psychology
Robert A. Wilson
Public health
Lindsey Grubbs
Race and racialism
Michael Billinger
Race betterment
Erna Kurbegovic
Race suicide
Adam Hochman
Racial hygiene
Frank W. Stahnisch
Racial hygiene and Nazism
Frank Stahnisch
Racial segregation
Paula Larsson
Racism
Michael Billinger
Reproductive rights
Erika Dyck
Reproductive technologies
Caroline Lyster
Residential schools
Faun Rice
Roles of science in eugenics
Robert A. Wilson
Schools for the Deaf and Deaf Identity
Bartlomiej Lenart
Science and values
Matthew J. Barker
Selecting for disability
Clarissa Becerra
Sexual segregation
Leslie Baker
Sexuality
Alexandra Minna Stern
Social Darwinism
Erna Kurbegovic
Sociobiology
Robert A. Wilson
Sorts of people
Robert A. Wilson
Special education
Jason Ellis
Speech-language pathology
Joshua St. Pierre
Standpoint theory
Joshua St. Pierre
Sterilization
Wendy Kline
Sterilization compensation
Paul Weindling
Stolen generations
Joanne Faulkner
Subhumanization
Licia Carlson
Today and Tomorrow: To-day and To-morrow book series
Michael Kohlman
Training schools for the feeble-minded
Katrina Jirik
Trans
Aleta Gruenewald
Transhumanism and radical enhancement
Mark Walker
Tuberculosis
Maureen Lux
Twin Studies
Douglas Wahlsten & Frank W. Stahnisch
Ugly Laws
Susan M. Schweik and Robert A. Wilson
Unfit, the
Cameron A.J. Ellis
Violence and disability
Dick Sobsey
War
Frank W. Stahnisch
Women's suffrage
Sheila Rae Gibbons

Environmentalism

Advocates of eugenic measures to enhance the quality of the human “stock” sometimes tried to win support for the cause by ridiculing or condemning a contrary view termed “environmentalism.” In this context, environmentalism is the belief that the most important reason people differ from each other in their thinking and behaviour is that their environments and upbringing differ. In the nature-nurture debate, it favours nurture and relegates nature to a minor position. Environmentalism views environment as the decisive factor that needs to be understood and enhanced in order to build a better world. It is the opposite of genetic determinism that attributes most differences among people to genetic differences inherited from the parents, a view that favours the “nature” side of the equation. Environmentalist beliefs pose a challenge to eugenics because the success of a eugenic policy requires that people low in ability or having intellectual defects suffer because of defects in heredity, not unfavourable environments. If bad environments are the principal cause of poor mental performance, social policies to improve the environments of children would be more effective. Eugenicists, however, generally opposed government measures to enhance the quality of life for the poor (Black, 2003; Chase, 1975).

Today, one rarely sees the term environmentalism used in this sense. Instead, the term is used to describe those who advocate for the health of the global environment and oppose changes wrought by human civilization that degrade the natural environment. In modern psychology and biology, it is difficult to identify anyone who self-identifies as an environmentalist and denies the importance of genetic factors in human development. Most of those who specialize in the study of environmental factors that are important for mental development nevertheless acknowledge the importance of genes for nervous system development and health (e.g., Gottlieb et al., 2006; Hood et al., 2010).

It is generally understood among scientists that two kinds of factors are involved in human development: heredity and environment. Heredity consists of all parts of the fertilized egg that are acquired from the parents at conception. Environment consists of things outside the individual that impinge on him or her. The 22 pairs of chromosomes and the X and Y chromosomes (girls are XX, boys are XY) contain most of the genes. The mitochondria in each cell also contain some genes, and they are inherited entirely from the mother. Genes tend to remain the same over a person’s lifetime, except when there is a rare mutation that suddenly alters a gene. Thousands of genes are known to be important for nervous system development and function (Maheu and Macdonald, 2011; Wahlsten, 2013). In most people most of these genes are normal and cannot be blamed for someone’s failures or credited for great successes.

Environment is very complex and can change rapidly. Global influences such as temperature and gravity are usually the same for all people living in the same locale. Substances from the environment that are ingested and digested as part of the diet or absorbed through the skin are assimilated to become part of the individual. The fertilized egg is microscopic, and the organism grows larger by converting things from the environment into its own body. There are also experiences via the sense organs (vision, hearing, smell, taste, touch) that provide vast amounts of information to the nervous system about the external world. Experiences can influence how the individual develops and alter the structure of the nervous system itself. The kinds of things we experience are governed by cultural factors and the social environment. Diets and cultural experiences differ greatly between members of most societies, and these vast differences can play a major role in psychological differences among people.

A belief that environment is critically important for child development does not conflict with what we know about genes (Wahlsten, 2002, 2003, 2013). Clearly, a mutation that causes a major defect in a gene can cause grossly abnormal development, but those kinds of defective genes are so harmful that they are usually not passed to the next generation. They are eliminated from the population by natural selection and therefore tend to be quite rare.

Eugenics as a political program was not aimed solely to reduce the frequency of rare defects of heredity. Instead, eugenics took aim at much more common problems such as low intelligence where no specific genetic cause could be identified. The possibility that poor environments were to blame for low intelligence posed a major challenge to the eugenics creed. Eugenicists in Canada answered that challenge simply by denying the role of a child’s environment and then ignoring it when making a decision about who should be sterilized. As documented in Leilani Muir’s life story (Muir, 2014), the Alberta Eugenics Board failed to consider adverse home environments when making their decisions. They believed as a matter of faith that genes were to blame and sterilization would halt the spread of bad genes.

Several decades of scientific research have shown that the distinction between what is hereditary and what is environmental is often not so clear. The early environment in mammals is provided mainly by the mother, and the maternal environment can function as a part of heredity. Its contribution can be examined by specialized analytical research methods (Carlier et al., 2000). It is also apparent that many influences in the early environment can alter the ways in which genetic information is expressed, and those alterations can be transmitted across generations. Such “epigenetic” phenomena are the focus of many recent studies, and evidence for their importance is accumulating (Burggren, 2014; Dias and Ressler, 2013; Stankiewicz et al., 2013). These findings show why one-sided views such as environmentalism and genetic determinism are no longer tenable.

-Douglas Wahlsten

  • Black, E. (2003). War against the weak. New York: Four Walls Eight Windows.

  • Burggren, W.W. (2014). Epigenetics as a source of variation in comparative animal physiology - or - Lamarack is lookin' pretty good these days. Journal of Experimental Biology, 217, 682-689.

  • Carlier, M., Roubertoux, P.L., & Wahlsten D. (2000). Maternal effects in behavior genetics analysis. In B. Jones & P. Mormède (Eds.), Neurobehavioural Genetics: Methods and Applications (187-197). New York: CRC Press.

  • Chase, A. (1975). The legacy of Malthus. New York: Knopf.

  • Dias, G.D., & Ressler, K.J. (2014). Parental olfactory experience influences behaviour and neural structure in subsequent generations. Nature Neuroscience, 17, 89-96.

  • Gottlieb, G., Wahlsten, D., & Lickliter, R. (2006). The significance of biology for human development: a developmental psychobiological systems view. In R. Lerner & W. Damon (Eds.), , 6th edition. New York: Wiley.

  • Hood, K.E., Halpern, C.T., Greenberg, G. & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.) (2010). Handbook of developmental science, behavior, and genetics. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers.

  • Maheu, L. & MacDonald, R. (Eds.) (2011). Challenging genetic determinism. Montreal: McGill-Queens University Press.

  • Muir, L. (2014). A Whisper past: childless after eugenics sterilization in Alberta. Friesen Press.

  • Stankiewicz, A.M., Swiergiel, A.H., & Lisowski, P. (2013). Epigenetics of stress adaptations in the brain. Brain Research Bulletin, 98, 76-92.

  • Wahlsten, D. (2013). A contemporary view of genes and behavior: complex systems and interactions. R.M. Lerner & J. Benson (Eds.), Advances in Child Development and Behavior. Amsterdam: Elsevier

  • Wahlsten, D. (2003). Genetics and the development of brain and behavior. In J. Valsiner & K.J. Connolly (Eds.), Handbook of Developmental Psychology (18-47). London: Sage.

  • Wahlsten, D. (2002). The theory of biological intelligence: history and a critical appraisal. In R. Sternberg & E. Gigorenki (Eds.), The General Factor of Intelligence: How General Is It? (245-277). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.